These 5-part short answer questions for TEFL/TESOL undergraduates are based on the lesson First Language Acquisition (L1): Stages and Processes.

Question 1 – First Language Acquisition (L1): Stages and Processes
a. List the major stages of L1 acquisition. b. What occurs during the telegraphic stage? c. How does babbling support future language development? d. How do children’s errors reflect rule development? e. What is the significance of overgeneralization? |
Answer
a.
The primary stages of first language acquisition are:
- Pre-linguistic Stage (0–12 months): Includes cooing and babbling, where infants experiment with sounds but do not produce recognizable words.
- Holophrastic (One-Word) Stage (12–18 months): Children use single words to express whole ideas (e.g., “milk” to mean “I want milk”).
- Two-Word Stage (18–24 months): Utterances consist of two words, often combining a noun and a verb or adjective (e.g., “more juice”).
- Telegraphic Stage (24–30 months): Speech resembles telegrams, using short, content-rich phrases without function words (e.g., “Daddy go work”).
- Later Multiword Stage (30+ months): Sentences become increasingly complex, incorporating function words and grammatical structures.
b.
During the telegraphic stage, children produce utterances that are short and omit non-essential words, much like a telegram. Their speech primarily includes content words such as nouns, verbs, and adjectives, while function words (articles, prepositions, auxiliary verbs) are largely absent. For example, a child might say “want cookie” instead of “I want a cookie.” This stage demonstrates the child’s growing grasp of basic syntax and the ability to combine words meaningfully.
c.
Babbling is crucial for future language development as it allows infants to practice articulatory movements and experiment with the sounds of their native language. Through babbling, children gain control over their vocal apparatus, develop phonemic awareness, and begin to recognize the prosodic patterns of speech. This stage lays the foundation for later word production by helping infants map sounds to meaning and refine their ability to produce language-specific sounds.
d.
Children’s language errors, such as overregularization (e.g., “goed” instead of “went”), reflect their active internalization and application of linguistic rules. These errors indicate that children are not merely imitating adult speech but are constructing and testing hypotheses about language structure. As they encounter exceptions and receive feedback, they gradually refine their understanding of grammatical rules, demonstrating cognitive engagement with language learning.
e.
Overgeneralization occurs when children apply regular grammatical patterns to irregular forms (e.g., “foots” instead of “feet”). This phenomenon is significant because it reveals the generative nature of language acquisition—the child’s ability to infer and extend rules beyond memorized forms. Overgeneralization is a normal, temporary phase that highlights the child’s developing linguistic competence and their progression toward mastering the complexities of their native language.
Question 2 – First Language Acquisition (L1): Stages and Processes
a. What role does imitation play in early language use? b. Define “caregiver speech” or “motherese.” c. How does input frequency affect language development? d. Why do children acquire grammar without formal instruction? e. What is the role of interaction in these stages? |
Answer
a.
Imitation is fundamental in early language use, serving as a primary mechanism through which infants and young children acquire new words, sounds, and communicative behaviors. By observing and replicating the speech patterns, intonation, and gestures of caregivers and others, children practice articulation, expand their vocabulary, and internalize the social norms of communication. Imitation also facilitates the mapping of linguistic forms to meanings, providing a foundation for later, more complex language development.
b.
Caregiver speech, also known as “motherese” or “child-directed speech,” refers to the distinctive way adults modify their language when interacting with young children. This speech style is characterized by:
- Higher pitch and exaggerated intonation
- Slower tempo and clearer articulation
- Simplified vocabulary and sentence structure
- Frequent repetition and emphasis on key words
These features make language more accessible and engaging for children, supporting their comprehension and encouraging participation in verbal exchanges.
c.
Input frequency—the number of times a child is exposed to specific words, phrases, or grammatical structures—significantly influences language development. High-frequency exposure increases the likelihood that children will notice, process, and eventually produce linguistic forms. Frequent input strengthens memory traces, aids in the acquisition of vocabulary and grammar, and accelerates the transition from passive understanding to active use. Conversely, limited exposure can delay or hinder the mastery of certain language elements.
d.
Children acquire grammar without formal instruction due to their innate capacity for pattern recognition and rule abstraction. Through exposure to natural language input, children subconsciously detect regularities and underlying structures, allowing them to generate and comprehend novel utterances. This process, often described as implicit learning, is supported by cognitive mechanisms that are particularly active in early childhood, enabling children to construct grammatical systems even in the absence of explicit teaching.
e.
Interaction plays a crucial role in all stages of early language development by providing meaningful, context-rich opportunities for communication. Through interactive exchanges with caregivers and peers, children receive immediate feedback, clarification, and scaffolding that facilitate comprehension and production. Interaction also supports joint attention, turn-taking, and pragmatic skills, ensuring that language learning is both social and functional. The dynamic nature of interaction helps tailor input to the child’s developmental level, maximizing learning potential.
Question 3 – First Language Acquisition (L1): Stages and Processes
a. Describe one universal feature of L1 acquisition across cultures. b. How does cognitive development relate to L1 development? c. In what way is language acquisition biological? d. How can L1 development inform L2 teaching practices? e. Describe one method for observing early language development. |
Answer
a.
A universal feature of first language (L1) acquisition is the developmental sequence of stages—from babbling to single words, then to two-word combinations, and finally to complex sentences. Regardless of the specific language or cultural context, children progress through these stages in a similar order and timeframe, demonstrating the innate and robust nature of language development.
b.
Cognitive development and L1 development are deeply interconnected. As children’s cognitive abilities mature—such as memory, attention, and symbolic thinking—they become better equipped to process, store, and manipulate linguistic information. For example, the emergence of object permanence and categorization skills supports vocabulary growth, while advances in abstract reasoning facilitate the understanding of grammar and more complex language structures.
c.
Language acquisition is biological in that humans are born with an innate predisposition for language learning, often referred to as the “language faculty” or “Universal Grammar.” This biological endowment enables children to extract linguistic rules from input and acquire any language they are exposed to, regardless of which language it is. Critical period effects and the universality of developmental milestones further support the biological basis of language acquisition.
d.
Insights from L1 development can inform second language (L2) teaching by emphasizing the importance of meaningful input, interaction, and developmental readiness. For example:
- Providing abundant, comprehensible input mirrors natural L1 exposure.
- Allowing for silent periods and gradual production respects individual developmental timelines.
- Recognizing common error patterns (such as overgeneralization) as signs of rule formation, not simply mistakes, can guide supportive feedback.
This perspective encourages L2 instruction that is more naturalistic, learner-centered, and responsive to developmental processes.
e.
Researchers or educators systematically record and analyze children’s spontaneous speech in everyday settings, such as at home or in preschool. This approach allows for the documentation of vocabulary emergence, syntactic structures, and communicative functions in authentic contexts, providing rich data on developmental patterns and individual differences.